Ask any experienced angler about their favorite part of fly-fishing, and you鈥檙e liable to get a single reply: the fight. To set your hook in a trout鈥檚 lip is to invite an arm-wrestling match against a subaquatic bowling pin made of primal rage. It鈥檚 why many of us fish.
In the midst of that adrenaline rush, it鈥檚 safe to say that few pause to wonder what it must feel like to be on the other end of the line, struggling against the tug of a metal hook impaled through your lip. Does it hurt? Do the creatures suffer when they begin to asphyxiate as they鈥檙e held out of water for the requisite photo?
These are the questions Brian Key sought to answer earlier this year. In a commentary for a new academic journal called Animal Sentience, the developmental neurobiology professor at the University of Queensland determined that fish lack the brain structure required for feeling pain. 鈥淲hat then do noxious stimuli鈥濃攖hat is, painful events, like getting pierced with a hook鈥斺渇eel like to a fish? The evidence best supports the idea that they don鈥檛 feel like anything to a fish.鈥
Key鈥檚 line鈥攖he same one grandfathers have told kids casting into lakes for generations鈥攎ay have seemed innocuous, but the opinion set the sleepy field of ichthyology, or fish zoology, on fire. His paper struck a nerve with animal pain researchers, garnering from academics around the world鈥攁nd all but three disagreed with Key鈥檚 assertions. compared Key鈥檚 argument to 鈥渁 ramshackle structure gaping with holes and pieced together from imperfectly understood neuroscience and often faulty literature citations.鈥
鈥淭he broad consensus from the scientific community is that fish most likely feel pain, and it is time governments display courage enough to act.鈥
鈥淧eople got very upset, and some of the commentaries were not kind,鈥 says Culum Brown, a behavioral ecologist who studies fish at Macquarie University in Sydney, Australia. 鈥淭he broad consensus from the scientific community is that fish most likely feel pain, and it is time governments display courage enough to act,鈥 .
The backlash shouldn鈥檛 surprise. In the past decade, researchers have produced a steady stream of evidence suggesting that fish do indeed feel pain. the animals have pain receptors in their brains, learn to , and show when confined in uncomfortable spaces. But actually proving what fish feel is nearly impossible. All scientists can do is poke or prod or shock them and observe their responses.
Assuming the overwhelming majority of researchers are right, the finding presents the millions of discerning catch-and-release recreationists and hundreds of millions of fish eaters around the world with a moral quandary: How much pain is too much for us to bear?
The answer could have real-world policy effects on the fishing industry, which pulls in between 1.7 and 3 trillion fish each year. Animal rights groups, such as PETA, argue that the haul amounts to animal cruelty. Closer to home, what does this mean for the piscavores of the world? Is there a pain-dependent case for choosing chicken or beef, which can be humanely raised and slaughtered, over fish? And what about the millions of us who find solace casting into the riffles of a deserted stream? Is sportfishing鈥攐r even catch-and-release fly-fishing鈥斺渁nimal abuse鈥 and 鈥渢orture,鈥 as fish advocate Mary Finelli claims?
The idea that fish are unthinking, unfeeling 鈥渓ower animals鈥 is an old one, at least to the days of Descartes, who argued that animals are not conscious beings. And although in the past century we鈥檝e come to accept that most mammals feel pain (ever step on your dog鈥檚 tail?), our views on fish haven鈥檛 budged. 鈥淢ost people are misinformed about these animals,鈥 says Jonathan Balcombe, author of the book What a Fish Knows.*
In some sense, this isn鈥檛 too surprising. After all, does any creature look as mindless as the fish, with its blank stare and dumb, gaping mouth? But in the past decade, fish鈥攆rom rainbow trout to grouper to goldfish鈥攈ave consistently awed scientists with their intelligence. Researchers the tusk fish using rocks to crack open clams, groupers with eels, and leopard sharks coming up to the surface for from trusted scientists.
Is there a pain-dependent case for choosing chicken or beef, which can be humanely raised and slaughtered, over fish?
And yet, societally, we treat fish differently than other animals that comprise the bulk of our dinner menus. A cow鈥檚 final moments walking down the kill chute are the subject of study and concern; McDonald鈥檚 vowed that all of its eggs will be from 鈥渃age-free鈥 chickens by 2025. 鈥淏ut we鈥檝e relegated fish to the cellar of the vertebrate group,鈥 says Balcombe, who also heads up animal sentience at the Humane Society Institute for Science and Policy.
Victoria Braithwaite, a professor of fisheries and biology at Penn State, has done as much as anyone to change the public鈥檚 perception of fish as creatures numb to our treatment of them. She conducted pioneering research in the early 2000s and published a groundbreaking book in 2010 called Do Fish Feel Pain? She is also refreshingly undogmatic on the subject. 鈥淚 have gone fishing, and my son fishes,鈥 Braithwaite says. 鈥淚 eat fish. That鈥檚 a choice we make. Part of our evolutionary background is that we eat other organisms鈥攚e are omnivorous. This is part of normal life.鈥
Braithwaite disagrees with Key鈥檚 assertions wholeheartedly. But, she says, evaluating pain in animals is not easy. Plenty of smaller creatures avoid harm as a survival instinct rather than fear of pain. And although we can perform experiments that cause fish harm, like injecting bee venom into a trout鈥檚 lips, we cannot tell exactly how it makes them feel. Because of this base uncertainty, the fish-feeling debate gets strangely deep, and fast. Sure, fish respond to 鈥渘oxious stimuli,鈥 in the industry parlance鈥攖hey writhe, they rock back and forth, they rub their stung lips on rocks鈥攂ut do they suffer? Are they conscious鈥攖hat is, capable of having thoughts and emotions about their suffering?
鈥淭he first thing to know is to realize 鈥榩ain鈥 is two processes,鈥 Braithwaite says. The first involves your nerve endings detecting damage, a trait all animals evolved to help them avoid injury. Think of it this way, Braithwaite says: 鈥淚f you pick up a hot cup, you drop it instantly, then you think about the pain a quarter of a second later.鈥 Your nerves immediately detected the damage, but you didn鈥檛 feel anything. It鈥檚 an automatic and inefficient process: You still get injured. That鈥檚 where the second part鈥攕uffering鈥攃omes in. 鈥淪omewhere along the evolutionary process,鈥 Braithwaite says, 鈥渨e developed the ow factor,鈥 the part of the response that actually hurts.
Showing the development of suffering鈥攖he 鈥渙w factor鈥濃攊n fish 鈥渋s much harder to prove,鈥 Braithwaite says. The field has relied on behavioral studies to see how fish react to pain. Case in point: Fish injected with acid into their lips appear to get stressed out. But when you give them a dose of morphine, they go right back to normal. In other words, they appear to suffer.
鈥淢any of us who are saying fish have this capacity are not saying that fish feel pain in the same way humans do,鈥 Braithwaite says. But fish have pain receptors in their brains, and dozens have studies have shown them responding negatively to and actively trying to avoid pain. 鈥淭o deny them an emotional response to damage that occurs? That seems really hard to do.鈥
Brown, the Australian behavioral ecologist, is fed up with the whole argument. The 鈥渄ebate鈥 reminds him of climate denial, he says, with a majority of scientists on one side and minority that seem unwilling to accept mounting evidence. 鈥淭he evidence, scientifically, is so overwhelmingly in favor鈥攖here might be some doubt, but not much more than on whether humans feel pain,鈥 Brown says. 鈥淗ow sure do you have to be before you decide it鈥檚 true? It鈥檚 a bit like gravity. It鈥檚 a technically theory, yes, but you don鈥檛 doubt it.鈥
If fish feel pain, then many of us may have to rethink our life choices. We catch and eat nearly a thousand times more fish than terrestrial animals, and fishing practices in much of the world are barbaric. Conventional fishing kills millions of fish unnecessarily and most certainly subjects the animals to pain. 鈥淚t really can鈥檛 be considered humane,鈥 says Mary Finelli of Fish Feel.
Why do some folks abstain from eating land animals but shovel down pounds of cod? Finelli鈥檚 not sure. The Humane Society Institute鈥檚 Balcombe is equally puzzled by pescatarians who forgo livestock for moral reasons. 鈥淭here isn鈥檛 any justification for eating a fish instead of chicken,鈥 he says.
When it comes to sportfishing, researchers who study fish pain are less understanding. 鈥淵ou鈥檙e talking about piercing an animal through the face,鈥 says Balcombe. 鈥淪ometimes through the eye. Probably a sensitive part of the body鈥攖hey need those organs to sense things and find food. Then you reel it in by its weight, and it gets hauled out by its weight if you don鈥檛 have a net, then there鈥檚 asphyxiation if it鈥檚 not put back into the water.鈥
鈥淚t鈥檚 not necessarily intentionally cruel, but from the fish鈥檚 perspective, it involves pain,鈥 he says. Brown, the Australian researcher, agrees. 鈥淚 think most people would be horrified of a picture of someone standing triumphantly on top of an elephant. But fishing is no different. You see trophy pictures of fish all the time. It鈥檚 just not treated the same way.鈥
Even the kind of fishing most Americans think of as humane鈥攃atch and release with barbless hooks, where the fish are released without leaving the water鈥攈as plenty of critics. A showed that about 89 percent of fish survived release, but their chances vary significantly based on the angler鈥檚 actions. Fish hooked anywhere but the lip have low survival rates鈥攕ea bass 鈥渄eep hooked鈥 in their bodies survive about 59 percent of the time if the hooks are removed from their bodies. Rainbow trout played for ten minutes have an 88 percent survival rate, but this if the fish is kept out of water for 30 seconds.
Jim Martin has thought long and hard about fish. He spent 30 years with the Oregon Department of Fish and Wildlife and was chief of fisheries for six years. He spent time working as conservation director for Pure Fishing, a global fishing gear conglomerate. In 2005, Martin was inducted into the National Freshwater Fishing Hall of Fame. He is a fisherman鈥檚 fisherman.
鈥淚鈥檝e always gone on the assumption on that fish do feel pain, just like a deer or a bird feels pain,鈥 he says. 鈥淭he issue of hunting and fishing is one that just has to be ethically and morally dealt with on the assumption that the animals do feel pain, rather than trying to discount that and say they don鈥檛 feel anything.鈥
There are also ways to fish that maximize the fish鈥檚 chance to live to fight another day. The Ministry of Natural Resources in Ontario, which studied a number of catch-and-release papers, suggests anglers stick to , refrain from using live bait (which often leads to deep hooking), and avoid fishing on warm days, when increased river temperatures lead to greater trout deaths.
Martin is unwilling to tell anyone how to live their lives, but for his part, he feeds his family with the fish he catches and takes solace in knowing the money he spends on permits goes toward conservation in his state.
鈥淓verybody has an ethical responsibility to decide how their activities are going to influence nature,鈥 he says. 鈥淲e鈥檙e all human beings. We all have values.鈥
*CORRECTION: A previous version of this article misstated the title of Jonathan Balcombe's book as What A聽Fish Feels.