国产吃瓜黑料

GET MORE WITH OUTSIDE+

Enjoy 35% off GOES, your essential outdoor guide

UPGRADE TODAY

If you buy through our links, we may earn an affiliate commission. This supports our mission to get more people active and outside. Learn more

woman of color with purple hair with outstretched arms while hiking
(Photo: Maria Hormeno Diaz/Getty)
Sweat Science

Getting Strong Is Good for Your Brain, Even if You Don鈥檛 Bulk Up

To ward off cognitive decline, a new analysis suggests that strength matters more than how much muscle you have

Published:  Updated: 
Image
(Photo: Maria Hormeno Diaz/Getty)

New perk: Easily find new routes and hidden gems, upcoming running events, and more near you. Your weekly Local Running Newsletter has everything you need to lace up! .

Resistance training has two jobs: to make you stronger and to make your muscles bigger. These two jobs are connected since bigger muscles are usually stronger. But they鈥檙e not identical. You can get stronger without adding muscle, for example, by improving how well signals are carried from your brain to your muscles or how effectively your existing muscle fibers are recruited. And you can add muscle without necessarily getting functionally stronger, which typically happens when you gain weight.

This distinction between strength and muscle is at the heart of a new study in the journal , which explores how each of these parameters influences the rate of cognitive decline in older adults. As a skinny endurance athlete who dutifully hits the gym, I鈥檝e long wondered whether my pull-ups and squats are pointless since I never seem to put on any muscle. But the new results, from a team led by Kristi Storoschuk of Queen鈥檚 University in Canada and Thomas Wood of the University of Washington, are reassuring.

Storoschuk and her colleagues pulled data from 1,424 adults over the age of 60 enrolled in the National Health and Nutrition Examination Study (NHANES) between 1999 and 2002. The subjects underwent DEXA scans to assess body composition, leg strength tests, a cognitive test called the , and various questionnaires that assessed things like physical activity habits. The DEXA scan was used to compute an 鈥渁ppendicular lean mass index鈥 (ALMI), which approximates how much muscle you have in your arms and legs, and a 鈥渇at-free mass index鈥 (FFMI), which reflects total muscle relative to height.

The key result was that strength was a much better predictor of cognitive performance than muscle mass. Strength explained about five percent of the variance in cognitive scores, while muscle mass explained only 0.5 percent. Moreover, whatever cognitive benefits muscle itself had seemed to be mediated by added strength. This isn鈥檛 as obvious as it sounds.

One theory is that muscle tissue should also be considered an 鈥,鈥 sucking up glucose from the bloodstream and secreting molecules called myokines that are thought to benefit health and brain function. In this view, simply having lots of muscle should protect against cognitive decline, regardless of how you use it. But the NHANES data didn鈥檛 support this view: muscle only mattered insofar as it made you stronger.

These findings agree with an earlier analysis of NHANES data (which I wrote about here) that explored the links between strength, muscle, and mortality. Like the current results, that study found that low strength raised the risk of premature death, but low muscle mass didn鈥檛. In contrast, a big study from McGill University did find that low muscle mass predicted more rapid cognitive decline over the three-year follow-up period, even after accounting for differences in strength.

The key result was that strength was a much better predictor of cognitive performance than muscle mass.

There are lots of possible explanations for the conflicting results, including different populations, different cognitive tests, and different sample sizes. As a result, it would be premature to conclude that muscle mass doesn鈥檛 matter at all.

Storoschuk and her colleagues suggest that there鈥檚 a distinction between muscle that you acquire from physical activity and muscle that you acquire in the process of gaining weight. In large population studies like NHANES, greater muscle mass may be predominantly a signal of larger body size rather than greater strength, which doesn鈥檛 seem to translate into protection from cognitive decline and other health benefits.

In terms of practical takeaways, I found the following figure from Storochuk鈥檚 paper interesting. It presents the relative benefits of several different variables on cognitive performance. The farther to the right each square is, the greater the cognitive benefits it鈥檚 associated with.

(Illustration: Lifestyle Medicine)

Sure enough, having low FFMI (i.e., low muscle mass) has no significant effect on cognitive scores, while peak leg force, a measure of strength, does have a significant benefit. What鈥檚 interesting to me is that the effect is even stronger for the next category, which is those who reported doing resistance training at least once per week. (The other categories below, like Vigorous Activities, include a mix of resistance and aerobic activity, so it鈥檚 hard to draw any conclusions about strength or muscle from them. Suffice it to say that aerobic exercise is also good for the brain.)

Here鈥檚 what I take from all this: having more muscle is very likely a good thing, and I鈥檒l keep trying to add some or at least avoid losing what I鈥檝e got. Getting stronger is even better, and I will draw motivation from the fact that, regardless of whether I鈥檓 adding muscle, I do see tangible gains over time in the weights I lift and the number of reps I can handle for a given exercise. But the most important and beneficial thing of all, judging from that graph above, is the simple fact that I鈥檓 doing resistance exercise in the first place.


For more Sweat Science, join me on and , sign up for the , and check out my book .

Popular on 国产吃瓜黑料 Online